| Home | E-Submission | Sitemap | Contact us |  
top_img
Sleep Med Res > Volume 16(1); 2025 > Article
Gumenyuk, Murman, Roth, Korzyukov, Miller, Parker, and Rizzo: Discrepancies Between Subjective and Objective Evaluation of Sleep: Potential Marker for Mild Cognitive Impairment

Abstract

Background and Objective

Discrepancies between objective and subjective evaluations of sleep efficiency have been observed in individuals with pathological and healthy aging. Objective sleep evaluation using actigraphy has been proposed as a potential tool for the clinical assessment of mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

Methods

Habitual sleep at home was evaluated using actigraphy (objective measure) and sleep diaries (subjective measure) in 45 participants (between 28 and 72 years old). Participants were divided into four groups by age and by diagnosis (MCI and Alzheimer desease). Cognitive and sleep measures were analyzed for comparisons and correlations.

Results

Significant discrepancies between objective and subjective sleep efficiency were observed in healthy and pathological ages. The MCI group showed the lowest sleep efficiency compared to other groups. Correlation analysis revealed a significant relationship between cognitive impairments and sleep efficiency in MCI and AD groups.

Conclusions

Objective sleep evaluation, with a particular focus on sleep efficiency, should be considered as a potential marker for MCI.

INTRODUCTION

While healthy sleep benefits the entire body [1], the most immediate and unavoidable consequences of sleep loss affect various cognitive functions [2].
Sleep quality and quantity change with both healthy and pathological aging. However, in healthy aging, the decline in sleep quality does not appear to impact cognitive functions [3], which is not the case in people with pathological aging. Thus, a recent review [4] concluded that dementia is associated with greater declines in sleep quality.
Strong evidence suggests that sleep alterations in Alzheimer disease (AD) and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients have a bidirectional relationship with cognitive impairments. It has been demonstrated that the relationship between poor sleep quality and neuronal atrophy could be a primary cause of cognitive impairments [5]. Importantly, poor sleep quality and sleep-wake disturbances are major risk factors for dementia and may serve as predictors for the onset of dementia years in advance [3]. Specifically, in MCI patients, sleep disorders are among the predominant symptoms [6,7].
Discrepancies between subjective (self-reported) and objective (actigraphy or polysomnography) measures of sleep quality in normally aging adults were highlighted [8,9]. A meta-analysis of examining quantitative sleep parameters in healthy participants aged 5–102 years [10] found that 15%–45% of healthy older adults reported difficulty initiating sleep, while actigraphy and polysomnography assessments indicated sleep quality issues in approximately 20%–65% of older participants.
Sleep quality, as defined by sleep efficiency (SE), influences daytime functioning and is crucial to overall well-being at all ages. In older adults, it can also indicate age-related changes that may progress to pathological levels [3,11,12].
SE can be evaluated through self-report methods with a validated sleep diary [13]. Despite the validation of this method, few studies have used it to assess self-reported sleep in older populations, including both healthy and pathological aging [14], perhaps due to inaccurate subjective assessments of sleep [15].
In contrast to the sleep diary, the objective measurement of sleep, such as using actigraphy, has been more widely applied in studies of healthy participants and much less on pathologically aged people [4,16]. These studies have reported mixed results when comparing healthy adults to patients with AD and MCI.
Thus, one study [17] found that the AD group showed lower SE and longer total sleep time (TST) compared to healthy older adults. Regarding MCI, four studies found no differences in sleep between MCI and control groups, while one study found lower SE in MCI patients compared to healthy controls [16].
To date, there are no clinical recommendations to use objectively measured SE as an indicator for MCI. Our study addresses this gap in the literature.
While the bidirectional relationship between aging and sleep changes are well-established, the question remains as to when these changes become pathological. These studies [10,18] demonstrate that sleep changes begin in early adulthood and progress across the lifespan in healthy people. Based on these observations, an important question is whether young to middle-aged adults can accurately evaluate their own sleep quality. This is crucial because sleep disturbances are known biomarkers of neurodegenerative disorders which can have an onset up to a decade before clinical symptoms appear.
Currently, the clinical literature does not recommend using objective measures of sleep in younger people, who are instead typically assessed by their subjective self-reports. Thus, in our study, we included a younger group to examine any discrepancies between objective and subjective sleep evaluations.
This study assessed SE using actigraphy recordings and sleep diary reports over 10–14 days in both normally aging individuals (aged 29–68 years) and pathologically aging individuals diagnosed with MCI or AD (aged 60–70 years). The primary aim was to identify any discrepancies between subjective and objective sleep evaluations across all participants, regardless of age, education, neurodegenerative diagnosis, or cognitive functioning level.

METHODS

Participants

This study was conducted with a limited budget allocated for pilot data collection. Sixty-four people initially responded to flyers and word-of-mouth advertisements. The initial advertisements did not specify inclusion or exclusion criteria. Based on our criteria, fifty-three individuals aged 25 and older qualified for the study. Patients diagnosed with various forms of dementia, including AD (13 patients), frontotemporal dementia (two patients), Lewy body dementia (three patients), and MCI (10 patients), were recruited through the Neurology Clinic at the University of Nebraska Medical Center. All interested participants were pre-screened for eligibility in a brief 5–10-minute phone conversation.
Forty-six participants passed the phone screening and were invited to provide written informed consent. For participants with pathological aging who might experience cognitive impairments, an additional consent form was provided to and signed by a legal guardian (a family member or close friend) who assisted with daily sleep-wake evaluations through subjective self-reports. The research coordinator maintained contact with both the participant and their legal guardian, providing reminders via phone calls to ensure accurate recording of the sleep diary and consistent use of the actiwatch throughout the study.
Exclusion criteria included major depression, primary sleep-wake disorders, significant hearing loss, and use of medications affecting sleep. After providing informed consent, participants underwent comprehensive cognitive assessments with version 3 of the Alzheimer Disease Centers’ Neuropsychological Test Battery in the Uniform Data Set (UDS-3 [19]) and a brief neurological examination and history conducted by an experienced neurologist (DLM) to rule out clinical signs or symptoms of stroke or parkinsonism using the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale part 3 motor score.

Neuropsychological Cognitive Assessment

The UDS-3 neuropsychological test battery was subdivided in 5 domains. Specifically, Memory (Benson recall, craft story), Visuospatial (Benson copy), Language (category fluency, The Multilingual Naming Test), Executive Function (trails B, letter fluency), and Attention (number span, trails A). Evidence of cognitive impairment in one of these 5 domains was met if either the Jak criteria [20] or the Peterson criteria [21] were met for that domain. This study includes only patients with AD and MCI, ranging from mild to moderate impairment severity across all cognitive evaluation questionnaires. To avoid potential confounding effects from demographic variables such as age, sex, and education in cognitive assessments, all cognitive evaluation results were normalized using the UDS-3 calculator (z-scores) (Table 1) [22].

Classification of Participants

The healthy control cohort was divided into two subgroups based upon age (young and older) (Table 1). The cognitive impaired groups (based on their cognitive clinical evaluations results) were divided into MCI or dementia based upon the severity of cognitive impairment and level of functional impairment determined with the neuropsychological test battery, Quick Dementia Rating Scale [23], and the Functional Activity Questionnaire [24]. The classification of MCI or dementia was consistent with the syndromic staging guidelines in the 2018 NIA-AA Research Framework [25]. Participants with dementia included in this report all met criteria for probable AD using the 2011 NIA-AA diagnostic guidelines for the clinical diagnosis of probable AD dementia [26]. Some AD patients had biomarker confirmation of their AD diagnosis, but others did not.
The final sample consisted of 45 participants: young age group (Y): 38.5±8.2 years, n=12, 6 females; older age group (O): 68.4±7.7 years, n=11, 6 females; MCI group: 70.1±7.4 years, n=12, 5 females; and AD group: 69.0±8.5 years, n=10, 5 females.
The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB #0887-21-EP, approval date: January 21, 2022) at the University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, Nebraska, USA. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants; for those in the MCI and AD groups, study partners acted as legally authorized representatives. All participants received $100 as compensation for their participation.

Subjective: Self-Report Sleep

All participants completed a sleep diary for two weeks in their home prior to the laboratory brain recording study (results to be reported elsewhere). If any participant had planned travel, the study was rescheduled to align with their habitual sleep-wake cycle. The self-reported sleep diary recorded bedtimes, rise times, sleep latency, number of awakenings, and total time spent WASO. From these variables, we calculated subjective time in bed (TIB), TST, and SE (TST/TIB×100).
The research coordinator called each participant every third day as a reminder to keep up with their daily sleep recordings. Participants were asked to complete the diary each morning with questions about the previous night’s sleep and each evening to record any medications taken, as well as the timing and duration of any naps. The sleep diary and actigraphy recordings began and ended simultaneously for each participant, allowing us to measure subjective and objective sleep data over the same period.

Objective: Actigraphy Sleep

To objectively measure sleep quality, we used the Actiwatch Spectrum Plus (Philips Respironics). Each participant wore a pre-programmed actiwatch at home for two weeks. The Philips Actiwatch provides reliable, previously validated measures of daytime movement activity and sleep parameters, including bed time, wake time, sleep latency, sleep duration, number of wake after sleep onset (WASO), and SE [27]. We used a minimum of 14 days of continuous actigraphy recordings—following [28]—to capture both day-to-day and week-to-week variability in habitual rest-activity patterns synchronized to the day-night cycle.
Participants were instructed to wear the actiwatch continuously on their non-dominant hand, except during water activities (e.g., showering, swimming) lasting more than 30 minutes. The actiwatch was programmed to record data for 30 days to ensure sufficient battery life and recording time for each participant. The actigraphy contains an accelerometer sensitive to movements of 0.025 g, which records physical motion in all directions. This motion is converted to an electrical signal, digitally integrated to derive an activity count per 30-second epoch. Data from the actiwatch were uploaded for offline analysis using Actiware software (Respironics, Inc.) after each participant returned to the laboratory with their recorded sleep-wake data.

Sleep Assessment with Questionnaires

All participants completed an extensive sleep interview and two standardized questionnaires: the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) and the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI). The ESS is a self-administered, eight-item questionnaire commonly used in clinical practice to measure daytime sleepiness [29]. It allows participants to report how often they inadvertently doze off during low-stimulation activities that involve being relatively immobile and relaxed. Older adults with frequently disrupted sleep often show increased daytime sleepiness as measured by the ESS [30].
The ISI is a seven-item self-report measure that assesses the severity of insomnia symptoms [31]. ISI scores range from 0 to 28, with higher scores indicating greater symptom severity. Both the ESS and ISI were administered.

Statistical Analysis

The primary aim of this study was to evaluate the discrepancy between objective and subjective sleep data in healthy participants and in patients diagnosed with MCI or AD. We analyzed actigraphy and sleep diary data for each participant, comparing objective and subjective sleep variables using paired-samples t-tests for dependent variables. For between-group comparisons of cognitive and sleep scores, we used one-way ANOVA, followed by post-hoc Scheffé tests when appropriate.
We assessed correlations between objective SE and cognitive executive function (as measured by the Trail Making Test, including both Digits and Letters) using Spearman’s rank-order correlation.

RESULTS

Cognitive Evaluation Results

Healthy participants across age groups scored within the normal range on the MoCA, with scores of 28 for Y-group and 27 for O-group. In contrast, the patient groups had scores within the pathological range, with MCI patients scoring a mean of 25 and AD patients scoring a mean of 20 (F[1, 20]=16.3; p=0.001). In the evaluation of attentional functions related to immediate recall (craft story), both patient groups recalled significantly fewer items compared to the healthy groups over 26 items (MCI: 19; AD: 6; F[1, 20]=19.8, p=0.002).
For attention assessed by the Trail Making Test, healthy participants completed the test faster than patient groups, with AD patients taking nearly twice as long as MCI patients (136 seconds vs. 74 seconds, respectively; F[1, 20]=5.7; p=0.02). Memory evaluation results indicated that AD patients had the lowest scores on both verbal (craft story delayed recall) and visual memory tasks (Benson Figure delayed recall) compared to MCI patients (F[1, 20]=41.9, p=0.001 for craft story delayed; F[1, 20]=14.0, p=0.001 for Benson Figure delayed). All scores, including the normalized z-scores (UDS-3) are presented in Table 1.

Sleep

Healthy participants

The results of objective and subjective sleep measures, including SE, are presented in Table 1. Overall, most participants accurately reported their TST based on sleep diary entries and actigraphy measurements (examples on Fig. 1), with unexpected discrepancies noted in the young healthy group. Fig. 2 illustrates the results for all groups in terms of SE, WASO, and latency to sleep onset.
In the young group, participants reported a longer TST in their sleep diaries (456 minutes) relative to what was recorded by actigraphy (425 minutes; t=3.04, p=0.01). Discrepancies between subjective and objective measures were also noted for SE, latency to sleep onset, and WASO. Specifically, young participants overestimated their SE in the sleep diary (94%) compared to actigraphy data (82%; t=6.1, p=0.005). Additionally, they underestimated WASO, reporting 13 minutes in their sleep diary versus 44 minutes recorded by actigraphy (t=-5.4, p=0.002).
In older healthy participants, discrepancies were found between subjective and objective results in SE (91% reported vs. 87% measured; t=2.7, p=0.02) and in nap duration (32 minutes reported vs. 67 minutes measured; t=-2.4, p=0.03).
Significant differences between younger and older participants were observed in objective measures of SE (82% for young vs. 87% for older; F[1, 2]=9.76, p=0.005) and latency to sleep onset (25 minutes for young vs. 14 minutes for older; F[1, 21]=7.14, p=0.01). These results indicate that younger participants showed significantly lower SE and longer sleep onset latency compared to older participants.

MCI and AD

In the MCI group, objective SE was significantly lower than self-reported SE (72.7% vs. 90%; t=4.1, p=0.001). Objective WASO was also significantly longer (31 minutes) than the self-reported WASO (21 minutes; t=-4.04, p=0.001).
In the AD group, objective SE was lower than self-reported SE as well (84% vs. 94%; t=3.2, p=0.01). Additionally, objective WASO in the AD group was significantly longer (43 minutes) compared to self-reported WASO (5 minutes; t=-4.66, p=0.001).
When MCI and AD participants were combined, objective SE was notably poorer than subjective reports (79% vs. 92%; F[1, 20]=22.6, p=0.002). Fig. 2 illustrates the significant discrepancies between objective and subjective SE in both the MCI and AD groups, with the MCI group showing worse objective SE than the AD group (F[1, 20]=5.06, p=0.035).
Objective WASO results did not show significant differences between the MCI and AD groups.
Correlation analysis examining the relationship between SE and central executive functioning (as measured by the Trail Making Test for Digits and Letters [32]) revealed that patients with lower objective SE performed significantly poorer on the Trail Making Test (Fig. 3). All other correlation analyses between sleep parameters and cognitive assessments showed no significant results.

Healthy aged vs. pathologically aged (MCI or AD)

Objective SE in healthy older participants was significantly higher than in the MCI group, though there was no significant difference between healthy participants and the AD group (main group effect: F[2, 30]=7.2, p=0.002). The subsequent Scheffe test confirmed a significant difference (p=0.004) specifically between the healthy and MCI groups.
Objective latency to sleep onset was longer in the MCI group compared to both healthy older participants and the AD group (main group effect: F[2, 30]=4.06, p=0.02). A subsequent Scheffe test indicated a significant difference (p=0.03) between the healthy older group and the MCI group.
Objective WASO did not show a significant difference between healthy older participants and patient groups.

Excessive daytime sleepiness and insomnia

Results from both questionnaires indicated no pathological daytime sleepiness and no insomnia in any of the groups (Table 1). There was a trend toward higher ESS scores in the MCI group (ESS=8) compared to the AD group (ESS=6). In the ISI score, the AD group showed a higher score (ISI=9) compared to the MCI group (ISI=7.3), though this difference did not reach statistical significance and was in the normal range.

DISCUSSION

In this study, we investigated discrepancies between subjective and objective measures of sleep quality across two healthy age groups—young and older adults—and two patient’s groups diagnosed with either MCI or AD. A surprising post-hoc finding emerged in the young group, revealing inaccurate self-reports about sleep and, generally unhealthy sleep habits. Specifically, we found that the young group had lower objective SE and longer sleep onset latency compared to the older healthy group. Also, the young group reported longer TST than was recorded in actigraphy data.
Our study was conducted with a very limited budget, as it is a pilot project that allowed us to enroll a relatively small sample size of participants. However, despite the small sample size of young participants in our study (n=12), the results regarding sleep quality are concerning and should prompt sleep professionals to address this unhealthy trend among younger people. Strong evidence exists for a bidirectional relationship between poor sleep quality and cognitive decline [33]. Given aging impact on sleep quality, it’s concerning how this detrimental combination could lead to significant health issues for these young people as they approach their 60s. The study [14] suggested that adults aged ≥55 years should ideally have sleep quality evaluated using actigraphy rather than self-report. In our study, we demonstrated that this discrepancy is starting to exist in healthy young people 25–38 years of old. In general, individuals in the preclinical stage of AD should be aware that sleep quality as defined by SE, rather than simply sleep quantity, is a predictor of amyloid deposition even before clinical symptoms of AD appear [34].
Our results on objective and subjective sleep quality, measured by SE, showed that all individuals in our sample (n=45)—regardless of age, education, sex, or cognitive functioning—were unable to accurately evaluate their sleep quality subjectively. Interestingly, all participants overestimated their sleep quality compared to objective measures. The MCI group showed the lowest objective SE (74%), consistent with previously published findings on the impact of MCI on sleep quality [6,7].
Correlation analysis in our study revealed a significant relationship between executive functioning and objective SE in patients diagnosed with neurodegenerative disorders (MCI or AD): slower performance on Trail Making Tests was associated with lower objective SE. Participants with MCI showed a longer TIB (mean 563 minutes) compared to patients with AD (mean 476 minutes), longer WASO (mean 31 minutes), and poorer SE compared to patients with AD (mean of 74% vs. 84%, respectively). These findings suggest that MCI pathology is associated with greater sleep disturbances as compared to participants diagnosed with AD. The underlying pathology of MCI affects not only cognitive function but also has a much greater effect on sleep quality than AD pathology [32]. This relationship requires further studies examining brain function and sleep-wake patterns in MCI using objective methods. Actigraphy could be considered a valuable and objective tool for evaluating sleep-wake rhythms in a habitual environment, providing insights into diurnal and nocturnal variability among patients with MCI and AD, as well as the differences between these two pathologies. Polysomnography is the more accurate objective tool for evaluating sleep, but it can only be used for a short period of time compared to actigraphy. In the recent systematic review [4], it was shown that only in four studies out for seven found poorer sleep quality in people with AD as compared to people diagnosed with MCI. Our results are in line with the studies that reported otherwise.
Although sleep has not been systematically examined in MCI as it was in AD, the sleep disruptions are frequently reported by patients with MCI and their caregivers [35]. In aphasic MCI and perhaps other neurological disorders, disrupted sleep may contribute to memory dysfunction. Memory issues in AD and MCI patients stem primarily from neuropathology in the medial temporal regions, with the hippocampus being the first and most extensively affected area in AD patients [35]. Structural brain changes due to MCI are less studied, but evidence suggests that the severity of dementia symptoms correlates with the severity of sleep disruptions [36].
Objective measures of sleep disturbances related to pathological aging can help identify the causes of dysregulated day–night behavior in individuals with abnormal aging. Specifically, as suggested by Ancoli-Israel et al. (2003) [37] actigraphy may be a valuable tool for objectively assessing the sleep–wake cycle and identifying circadian misalignment associated with neurodegeneration. It may help older people who are living with their caregivers to correct their sleep-wake and circadian rhythms accordingly to their home-established environment.
In older adults who consider themselves normally aged, age-related sleep disturbances may lead to low SE and can have serious consequences, such as falls, difficulties with walking, movement, and vision, and may contribute to age-related depression [38]. In our study, older participants who considered themselves as healthy had lower WASO and shorter sleep onset latency than both the young control group and the pathologically aging group. The sleep–wake rhythms in the older control group appeared healthier compared to those of other participants. Additionally, 28% of participants initially assigned to the control older group were diagnosed with MCI after clinical and physical evaluation by a neurologist in our research study. That said, the “continuum” of pathological aging could be underdiagnosed in individuals aged 60 and older until their sleep and cognitive functions are measured using objective tools.
Diminished cognitive functioning, particularly in attention and memory, is concerning in older adults, as it may be mistaken for dementia. Significant changes in cognition and sleep habits can also lead to early institutionalization and loss of independence in daily activities [39,40]. Therefore, a thorough evaluation of sleep habits—especially using objective tools—should be included in clinical assessments of older adults. Actigraphy-measured SE could serve as a marker for sleep disturbances in individuals over 60 who report attention and memory impairments. Healthcare professionals working with the geriatric population need to learn to differentiate the different causes of sleep disturbances in this population so that appropriate therapy can be initiated.
In summary, our study provided evidence of discrepancies between objective and subjective sleep assays in young, old, and neurodegenerative disorder patients. Patients diagnosed with MCI showed worse objective SE compared to those with AD. SE may serve as a specific marker to differentiate between these two overlapping pathologies. Future studies are needed to explore the association between brain structural changes and sleep quality in people with MCI disorder.

NOTES

Availability of Data and Material
The datasets generated or analyzed during the study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Valentina Gumenyuk, Thomas Roth, Daniel L Murman, Oleg Korzyukov. Data curation: Valentina Gumenyuk, Sheridan M Parker, Nicholas R Miller. Formal analysis: Valentina Gumenyuk, Daniel L Murman, Oleg Korzyukov, Sheridan M Parker. Funding acquisition: Matthew Rizzo. Investigation: Gumenyuk Valentina, Daniel L Murman, Thomas Roth. Methodology: Valentina Gumenyuk, Daniel L Murman. Project administration: Valentina Gumenyuk. Resources: Matthew Rizzo. Software: Valentina Gumenyuk, Daniel L Murman. Supervision: Valentina Gumenyuk. Validation: Valentina Gumenyuk, Daniel L Murman, Thomas Roth. Visualization: Valentina Gumenyuk, Oleg Korzyukov, Daniel L Murman. Writing—original draft: Valentina Gumenyuk. Writing—review & editing : Valentina Gumenyuk, Daniel L Murman, Thomas Roth, Oleg Korzyukov, Sheridan M Parker.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
Funding Statement
None
Acknowledgements
Authors thank all participants for their contribution to this study. We also extend our gratitude to Thristan Jones for his assistance with data collection. Authors thank Ren Haasch for her work on UDS-3 z-score calculations in this study. VG received research funding from the NIGMS, P20GM130447, Cognitive Neuroscience and Development of Aging (CoNDA) Award.

REFERENCES

1. Knutson KL, Spiegel K, Penev P, Van Cauter E. The metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Sleep Med Rev 2007;11:163-78.
crossref pmid pmc
2. Zamore Z, Veasey SC. Neural consequences of chronic sleep disruption. Trends Neurosci 2022;45:678-91.
crossref pmid pmc
3. Lim AS, Kowgier M, Yu L, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Sleep fragmentation and the risk of incident Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive decline in older persons. Sleep 2013;36:1027-32.
crossref pmid pmc
4. Casagrande M, Forte G, Favieri F, Corbo I. Sleep quality and aging: a systematic review on healthy older people, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2022;19:8457.
crossref pmid pmc
5. St Louis EK, Boeve BF. REM sleep behavior disorder: diagnosis, clinical implications, and future directions. Mayo Clin Proc 2017;92:1723-36.
crossref pmid pmc
6. McKinnon A, Terpening Z, Hickie IB, Batchelor J, Grunstein R, Lewis SJ, et al. Prevalence and predictors of poor sleep quality in mild cognitive impairment. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2014;27:204-11.
crossref pmid
7. Hayes TL, Riley T, Mattek N, Pavel M, Kaye JA. Sleep habits in mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2014;28:145-50.
crossref pmid pmc
8. Ancoli-Israel S, Kripke DF. Prevalent sleep problems in the aged. Biofeedback Self Regul 1991;16:349-59.
crossref pmid
9. Benca RM, Teodorescu M. Chapter 26 - Sleep physiology and disorders in aging and dementia. Handb Clin Neurol 2019;167:477-93.
pmid
10. Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, Vitiello MV. Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 2004;27:1255-73.
crossref pmid
11. Vitiello MV, Borson S. Sleep disturbances in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: epidemiology, pathophysiology and treatment. CNS Drugs 2001;15:777-96.
crossref pmid
12. Anderson KN, Bradley AJ. Sleep disturbance in mental health problems and neurodegenerative disease. Nat Sci Sleep 2013;5:61-75.
crossref pmid pmc
13. Carney CE, Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, Krystal AD, Lichstein KL, et al. The consensus sleep diary: standardizing prospective sleep self-monitoring. Sleep 2012;35:287-302.
crossref pmid pmc
14. Landry GJ, Best JR, Liu-Ambrose T. Measuring sleep quality in older adults: a comparison using subjective and objective methods. Front Aging Neurosci 2015;7:166.
crossref pmid pmc
15. Westerberg CE, Lundgren EM, Florczak SM, Mesulam MM, Weintraub S, Zee PC, et al. Sleep influences the severity of memory disruption in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: results from sleep self-assessment and continuous activity monitoring. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2010;24:325-33.
pmid pmc
16. Alfini A, Albert M, Faria AV, Soldan A, Pettigrew C, Wanigatunga S, et al. Associations of actigraphic sleep and circadian rest/activity rhythms with cognition in the early phase of Alzheimer’s disease. Sleep Adv 2021;2:zpab007.
crossref pmid pmc
17. Liguori C, Romigi A, Nuccetelli M, Zannino S, Sancesario G, Martorana A, et al. Orexinergic system dysregulation, sleep impairment, and cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol 2014;71:1498-505.
crossref pmid
18. Roffwarg HP, Muzio JN, Dement WC. Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science 1966;152:604-19.
crossref pmid
19. Weintraub S, Besser L, Dodge HH, Teylan M, Ferris S, Goldstein FC, et al. Version 3 of the Alzheimer Disease Centers’ Neuropsychological Test Battery in the Uniform Data Set (UDS). Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2018;32:10-7.
crossref pmid pmc
20. Jak AJ, Preis SR, Beiser AS, Seshadri S, Wolf PA, Bondi MW, et al. Neuropsychological criteria for mild cognitive impairment and dementia risk in the Framingham Heart Study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2016;22:937-43.
crossref pmid pmc
21. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med 2004;256:183-94.
crossref pmid
22. Dodge HH, Goldstein FC, Wakim NI, Gefen T, Teylan M, Chan KCG, et al. Differentiating among stages of cognitive impairment in aging: Version 3 of the Uniform Data Set (UDS) neuropsychological test battery and MoCA index scores. Alzheimers Dement (N Y) 2020;6:e12103.
crossref pmid pmc
23. Galvin JE. The Quick Dementia Rating System (QDRS): a rapid dementia staging tool. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2015;1:249-59.
crossref pmid pmc
24. González DA, Gonzales MM, Resch ZJ, Sullivan AC, Soble JR. Comprehensive evaluation of the Functional Activities Questionnaire (FAQ) and its reliability and validity. Assessment 2022;29:748-63.
crossref pmid pmc
25. Jack CR Jr, Bennett DA, Blennow K, Carrillo MC, Dunn B, Haeberlein SB, et al. NIA-AA Research Framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2018;14:535-62.
pmid
26. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011;7:263-9.
pmid
27. Dick R, Penzel T, Fietze I, Partinen M, Hein H, Schulz J. AASM standards of practice compliant validation of actigraphic sleep analysis from SOMNOwatch™ versus polysomnographic sleep diagnostics shows high conformity also among subjects with sleep disordered breathing. Physiol Meas 2010;31:1623.
crossref pmid
28. Van Someren EJ. Melatonin treatment efficacy: for whom and for what? Sleep Med 2007;8:193-5.
crossref pmid
29. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 1991;14:540-5.
crossref pmid
30. Stepanski E, Lamphere J, Badia P, Zorick F, Roth T. Sleep fragmentation and daytime sleepiness. Sleep 1984;7:18-26.
crossref pmid
31. Bastien CH, Vallières A, Morin CM. Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med 2001;2:297-307.
crossref pmid
32. Reitan RM. Validity of the Trail Making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills 1958;8:271-6.
crossref
33. Köhler S, Soons LM, Tange H, Deckers K, van Boxtel MPJ. Sleep quality and cognitive decline across the adult age range: findings from the Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS). J Alzheimers Dis 2023;96:1041-9.
crossref pmid pmc
34. Ju YE, McLeland JS, Toedebusch CD, Xiong C, Fagan AM, Duntley SP, et al. Sleep quality and preclinical Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol 2013;70:587-93.
crossref pmid pmc
35. Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Hudon C. Sleep disturbances in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr 2009;21:654-66.
crossref pmid
36. Pat-Horenczyk R, Klauber MR, Shochat T, Ancoli-Israel S. Hourly profiles of sleep and wakefulness in severely versus mild-moderately demented nursing home patients. Aging (Milano) 1998;10:308-15.
crossref pmid
37. Ancoli-Israel S, Cole R, Alessi C, Chambers M, Moorcroft W, Pollak CP. The role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep 2003;26:342-92.
crossref pmid
38. Brassington GS, King AC, Bliwise DL. Sleep problems as a risk factor for falls in a sample of community-dwelling adults aged 64-99 years. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000;48:1234-40.
crossref pmid
39. Tinetti ME, Williams CS. Falls, injuries due to falls, and the risk of admission to a nursing home. N Engl J Med 1997;337:1279-84.
crossref pmid
40. Gaugler JE, Edwards AB, Femia EE, Zarit SH, Stephens MA, Townsend A, et al. Predictors of institutionalization of cognitively impaired elders: family help and the timing of placement. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2000;55:P247-55.
crossref pmid

Fig. 1.
Actogram of day-night rhythm for four representative participants from each group recorded at home. Each row of the actogram represents a 24-hour period starting at 12 PM. The participant’s sleep periods are indicated by blue shading. Activity (movements) periods are depicted by black vertical deflections. Activity during sleep periods is associated with sleep fragmentation and awakenings. Dark blue vertical deflections indicate times when the actiwatch was not in use (e.g., during water-related activities). MCI, mild cognitive impairment; AD, Alzheimer disease.
smr-2025-02684f1.jpg
Fig. 2.
Discrepancies between objective and subjective sleep parameters in Y, O, MCI, and AD groups across sleep efficiency, WASO, and latency to sleep onset. Statistical differences are reported in the text. *p<0.01. Y, young age group; O, old age group; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; AD, Alzheimer disease; WASO, wake after sleep onset.
smr-2025-02684f2.jpg
Fig. 3.
Illustrates the relationship between executive functions and sleep efficiency in people diagnosed with MCI and AD. MCI, mild cognitive impairment; AD, Alzheimer disease.
smr-2025-02684f3.jpg
Table 1.
Demographic, cognitive and sleep (objective and subjective) results for healthy (young and old), patients with MCI and AD
Characteristics Young (n=12) Old (n=11) MCI (n=12) AD (n=10) p-value
Age (yr) 38.5±8.3 70.9±6.6 70.7±7.4 69.0±8.5 O, MCI, AD: n.s.
Female 6 (50) 6 (55) 5 (45) 5 (50) n.s.
Craft story (immediate) 30.5±1.5 26.3±6.9 19.0±6.5 6.6±6.5 O>MCI>AD: p=0.01
UDS-3 z-score (craft story) NA 0.3±0.9 -0.3±1.2 -2.8±0.4
Benson figure (immediate) 13.9±2.3 12.3±2.4 9.8±4.0 2.9±4.3 Y, O: n.s; MCI>AD: p=0.01
UDS-3 z-score (Benson figure) NA 0.3±0/9 -0.5±1.3 -2.9±1.4
Trail making test (Digits [sec]) 23.0±5.8 29.7±9.3 35.2±19.9 51.1±27.6 Y, O<AD: p=0.002
UDS-3 z-score (Trail [digits]) NA 0.1±0.6 -0.5±2.0 -2.0±2.6
Trail making test (Digits and Letters [sec]) 47.3±23 58.4±21 74.6±48 136.4±72.5 Y, O<MCI, AD: p=0.02
UDS-3 z-score (Trail [Digits and Letters]) NA 0.6±0.5 0.08±1.5 -1.4±1.8
Craft story (delayed) 29.0±8.2 23.0±5.8 16.5±7.2 1.3±1.9 Y, O, MCI>AD: p=0.001
Benson figure (delayed) 13.9±2.3 12.6±2.3 9.6±3.9 2.9±4.4 Y, O, MCI, AD: p=0.001
Digit span test (forward) 11.3±1.7 11.1±2.0 9.7±2.1 9.7±2.0 n.s.
Digit span test (backward) 6.9±2.7 8.3±2.3 5.7±2.1 5.6±2.5 n.s.
MoCA 28.7±1.5 27.5±1.6 24.8±2.2 19.8±3.6 Y, O, MCI>AD: p=0.04
Actigraphy TST (min) 425.3±60 445.3±61 453.8±62 429.2±104 n.s.
Actigraphy SE (%) 82.6±4.5 87.8±3.3 74.3±15.1 84.3±7.9 Y<O: p=0.02; MCI<AD: p=0.01
Actigraphy WASO (min) 44.4±15 40.3±12 52.0±24 43.7±24 Y vs. O: n.s.; MCI>AD: p=0.05
Actigraphy sleep latency onset 25.1±14 13.7±4.0 27.9±14 22.4±15.5 Y>O: p=0.01; MCI vs. AD: n.s.
Sleep log TST (min) 456.0±58 433.4±57 448.8±41 442.6±64 Y vs. O: n.s.; MCI vs. AD: n.s.
Sleep log SE (%) 93.8±3.6 90.9±3.5 91.0±5.4 94.2±4.4 n.s.
Sleep log WASO (min) 13.5±10 31.1±18 21.5±20 4.9±6 Y<O: p=0.008; MCI>AD: p=0.005
Sleep log sleep latency onset 13.6±9.6 12.4±6.0 22.5±14 17.3±16 Y vs. O: n.s.; MCI vs. AD: n.s.
Epworth Sleepiness Scale 6.0±2.5 4.0±2.5 8.0±4.5 5.9±3.0 Y vs. O: n.s.; MCI vs. AD: n.s.
Insomnia Severity Index 5.3±3.7 7.0±3.0 7.3±3.6 9.0±5.4 Y vs. O: n.s.; MCI vs. AD: n.s.

Values are presented as mean±standard deviation or number (%).

UDS-3, version 3 of the Alzheimer Disease Centers’ Neuropsychological Test Battery in the uniform data set; TST, total sleep time; SE, sleep efficiency; WASO, wake after sleep onset; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; AD, Alzheimer disease; Y, young age group; O, old age group; n.s., not significant; NA, not applicable.

TOOLS
PDF Links  PDF Links
PubReader  PubReader
ePub Link  ePub Link
XML Download  XML Download
Full text via DOI  Full text via DOI
Download Citation  Download Citation
  Print
Share:      
METRICS
0
Crossref
0
Scopus
294
View
9
Download